The family dog is probably in synch with your children

Family dogs match their movements to those of the children they live with, according to a poignant new study of young people and their pets. In the study, pet dogs moved when their accompanying children did and remained still when they stopped, a physical synchrony that often signals emotional bonding. The family canines also tended to stay close by and to orient themselves in the same directions as the kids, a further indication of social engagement and attentiveness that could have implications for the emotional development of both dogs and youngsters, as well as for the safety of the interactions between them.

The results add to the growing evidence that how people and other creatures move depends to a surprising extent on who they are with, and that social connections can be shaped and strengthened by shared activity. The findings also raise practical questions about how children and dogs can best learn to read each other's body language, and how family pets might help to encourage children to move more or best serve as a source of emotional support.

The idea that being around others influences how we move is not new. Past research shows that romantic couples tend unconsciously to synchronize their walking pace to a much greater extent than strangers do and that men often speed up when walking with other men, even if the new pace is not physiologically comfortable for them.

In the same way, moving together seems to build familiarity, even if it did not exist before. People who take up dancing together, for instance, often express greater closeness and cohesion afterward. Moving in tandem seems to generate intangible, intimate bonds.

Other research suggests that these connections are not limited to humans. When, during earlier studies, scientists asked adults to bring their pet dogs to large, unfamiliar rooms and then walk around the space, sometimes pausing, sometimes changing direction, and not directly engaging with their pets, the dogs followed and repeated their owners' actions to a remarkable degree. About 80% of the time, the dogs walked when their owners walked and fell still when they stopped. They also usually turned and pivoted whenever their owners did, so that they stayed oriented in the same direction.

Dogs in shelters likewise aped the actions of their primary caregivers, other studies showed, but to a lesser extent than among family dogs, indicating looser bonds. Little was known, though, about whether pet dogs similarly synchronize themselves with their family's children. So for the new study, which was published in January in Animal Cognition, researchers at Oregon State University in Corvallis turned to a group of previously assembled volunteers. As part of a planned, long-term study of animal-assisted interventions for children with developmental differences, the scientists already had recruited local families with children age 8 to 17 and their pet dogs.

They now asked 30 of the children, some with developmental differences, such as autism, and some without, if they would bring their dogs to a large, warehouselike space. The dogs, ranging in breed from floppy retrievers to a Jack Russell mix, a whippet, several poodles and a Great Dane, had received no special training at the time. They were pets.

The researchers asked the youngsters to follow pathways marked with tape in the cavernous space, pausing sometimes, and switching directions, without touching or talking to their dogs. The researchers filmed and later analyzed each duo's interactions and found that the dogs stayed close to their young owners, walking or stilling in tune with them more than 60% of the time. The dogs also turned frequently to face in the same direction as the child, like greenhorn square dancers.

Their synchrony was less than researchers had noted earlier among adults and their dogs but far greater than would be expected by chance, the researchers concluded.

"The dogs clearly were paying attention and adjusting their behavior" in response to the actions of the child, said Monique Udell, an associate professor of animal sciences at Oregon State and senior author of the study.

Udell and her colleagues also are interested in studying the bonding and interwoven movements of people and other types of pets, particularly cats. "We've done a little work with cats and, so far, they blow everything out of the water in terms of being socially responsive to their owners' behavior," she said. No experiments currently are planned, however, to test the synchrony of cats and dogs.

Upcoming Events